====== Dadibi ====== Timothy Usher, Santa Fe Institute ===== Situation ===== Dadibi is spoken by 5,000 to 6,000 people (1974) living around the Karimui Plateau in Papua New Guinea's Simbu district and in three villages on the Erave River in Southern Highlands province. It may be divided into Karimui Plateau and Erave River dialects (MacDonald 1973: 114-116, Mac Donald and Mac Donald 1974: 129-130.) ===== Sources ===== Franklin (1968) Gulf District MacDonald (1973) Teberan family MacDonald and MacDonald (1974) phonology MacDonald (1976) grammar MacDonald (1992) sketch phonology MacDonald survey vocabulary provided in digitalized form by Paul Whitehouse via Summer Institute of Linguistics in Ukarumpa Whitby and Bai (1990, 2008) dictionary ===== Phonology ===== MacDonald and MacDonald (1974: 131) give 13 consonants and 5 vowels for Dadibi as follows: |< - 60px 60px 60px 60px >| | m | n | | | | p | t | | k | | b | d | | g | | | s | | h | | w | l | j | | |< - 60px 60px 60px >| | i | | u | | e | | o | | | a | | Vowels are further distinguished by the presence vs. absence of nasalization: |< - 60px 60px >| | i | ĩ | | e | ẽ | | a | ã | | o | õ | | u | ũ | As is typical of languages in the north Papuan Gulf region, initial stops /b d g/ are realized as plain voiceless [p t k]. These contrast with aspirated stops /p t k/ [pʰ tʰ kʰ], which in native words occur only initially (1974: 132, 134-135.) The contrast between velars /k h/ arose from a split in the reflexes of /*k/, which became /h/ when followed by vowels /*a *ɔ *o/ but remained /k/ when followed by /*i *e *u/, a split which finds its typological analogs in Awyu and Mian reflexes of /*k/. As in Awyu, the contrast became phonemic due to loans, e.g. /kone/ "think/know" from Southern Highlands /*kone/. Both /k h/ occur in native words only root-initially. ===== Pronouns ===== MacDonald (1973: 126-127, 1976: 45-49) gives Dadibi pronouns in three case forms as follows: |< - 100px 100px 100px 100px >| | |absolutive |ergative/poss.|reflexive poss.| |1 sg.|éná |en-o |ená | |2 sg.|nagi |nag-o |naga | |3 sg.|ãgã |ãgã-ĩ́ |ã́gã | |1 pl.|da |da-go |dena | |2 pl.|dagi |dagi-go |duga | |3 pl.|augʷa-di ≈ -li|augʷa-li-go |augʷa | Those in the first set are absolutive in the strict sense, indicating subjects of intransitive verbs and objects of transitive verbs, those in the second functioning as agent subjects of transitive verbs and possessors. The same division of function is found in Fasu (Loeweke and May 1966: 26) and Rumu (Petterson 1989 ibid.) as well as Folopa (Anderson 2010: 19-24.) The third set, which MacDonald dubs reflexive possessives, indicate the subject of verbs with locative or patient arguments possessed by the subject. MacDonald shows the ergative-possessive plurals to be derived from the absolutives by the encliticization of /-go/, which marks ergative-possessive regular nominals. When the pronouns is qualified by a numeral, it is appended thereto instead; e.g. /da si/ "we two (abs.)" /da si-go/ "we two (erg./poss.)." The first and second person singulars are marked by /-o/ rather than /-go/. The reflexive possessive form of the third person plural is original, the absolutive form being derived from it by the addition of a suffix /-di/ [-di -li]. ===== Verbal morphology ===== **[under construction]**\\ \\ MacDonald (1973: 57-69)…\\ \\ Person is distinguished only in imperatives, as follows: (pp. 66-69): |< - 100px 100px 100px >| | |imperative |prohibitive| |2 sg. |-a(-o/-ma) |-gi-o | |2 pl. |-ia(-o/-ma) |-gimi-o | |1 pl. |-ina(-o/-ma)|--- | |3 sg./pl.|-ma-ina-o |--- | MacDonald identifies the suffixes /o/ and /ma/ as indicative or vocative and emphatic respectively, though qualifying that /ma/ occurs only with imperatives.\\ \\