Riandana

Timothy Usher, Santa Fe Institute

Situation

[under construction]

Riandana (Riantana) is spoken by 1,000 people (1949) living in six villages in the northernmost portion of Kolopom (Frederick-Hendrik) island. Drabbe names these villages as Ambébé (Iramoro,) Roàmbu (Soam,) Awïra (Kundjubandjung,) Kāmba, Kòmöka and Jarà. The term /rja-ndana/ [ria-nˈtana rian̪d̪ana] means “real people,” in contrast to outsiders who are called Kwökö /kwʏkʏ/ (Drabbe 1949: 1-2, 3., Choi 2015: …)

Sources

Drabbe (1949) brief description of and (pp. 14-24) 469 comparative terms for Riantana

Drabbe (1950) comparative vocabulary of Riantana

Drabbe (1954: 231-255) 100 comparative terms for Riantana

Voorhoeve (1975: 365-366) phonological inventory and pronouns for Riantana following Drabbe (1949)

Voorhoeve (1975: 96) 40 comparative terms for Riantana following Drabbe (1949)

Menanti and Susanto (2001) survey of Kimaam (unobtained)

Choi (2015) grammar of and (pp. 55-507) 203 terms for Riantana

In addition to these, a vocabulary of Riantana was provided in digitalized form by Paul Whitehouse via the Summer Institute of Linguistics; however it is undated and unattributed.

Phonology

[under construction]

Drabbe (1949: 3-4) gives 19 consonants and 7 vowels for Riantana as follows:

m n
p t tr k
b d dr
mb nd ndʲ ŋg
s
β
w r j
i u
ʏ
e o
ɛ
a

It is clear from comparison to Choi's materials that Drabbe's /tr dr/ are merely clusters which Drabbe considered to be phonemic because they are the only recurring unambiguous clusters in the language.

Unlike Choi (2015) (below) who explicitly states that the laminal stops are not affricates, Drabbe isn't clear about the precise qualities of the stops written as <tj ndj>. Usually in Drabbe's work these graphemes would denote affricates [tʃ ndʒ], but his general comment about Kolopom <dj> (“De medeklinker dj echter heeft een duidelijke d -klank, juist als bv. in het Maleise djambu ”) can be read as supporting Choi's interpretation; accordingly these sounds are shown as <tʲ ndʲ> herein.

Drabbe's <ö> is specified as phonetic [ʏ] (Dutch <stuk>) in Riantana. We show it as such here rather than as <ə> because it sometimes corresponds to [u] …


Choi (2015: 16-26) gives a somewhat different inventory with 22 or 23 consonants and 7 vowels:

m n ŋ
p t [ʈ] c k
b d ɟ g
mb n̪d̪ nd ɲɟ ŋg
β ɣ
w r j
i u
ə o
ɛ ɔ
a

Choi's retroflexed voiceless stop /ʈ/ is found in only a few words (2015: 18-19) where she alleges it to be contrastive with apical /t/. but seems uncertain of its phonemic validity.

Choi's velar non-stop /ɣ/, which Drabbe did not recognize at all, is evidently uncommon, being found medially in only six words in her grammar and termlist ….

….

Based upon a review of the available materials, we propose 14 to 18 consonants and 6 vowels for Riandana, with marginal or debatable components given in brackets:

m n
p t k
b d [dʲ] [g]
mb nd [ndʲ] ŋg
w r j [ɣ]
i u
ʏ
o
ɛ
a

Final consonants are not found in free forms, a few examples in Drabbe's (1949: 14-24) termlist probably being attributable to devoicing of final vowels as described in Choi (2015: 24-25.) However, underlying final consonants /m n r/ and possibly /t/ which continue those of Proto-Kolopom are discernible when the root is followed by a suffix (below):

m n
[t]
r

Somewhat unsuually for New Guinean languages, clusters involving non-stops /w r j/ as their second member are common both initially and mdially. This analysis is favored over one which treats /CwV CjV/ as sequential vowels /CuV CiV/ because clusters /trV drV/ are also found and because there are otherwise no sequential vowels. The following combinations have been observed in Drabbe's and Choi's data:

w r j
m mw mj
p pw
b
n nw nj
t tr
d dw dr
k kw
r rw rj
j jw

Thus Riandana roots are of the structure /(C(C₂))V(C₃)/, /(C(C₂))VC(C₂)V(C₃)/, etc., where /C₂ C₃/ are members of restricted sets as outlined above.


While Choi's Riantana probably represents the same dialect as Drabbe's, Choi's transcription sometimes differs from Drabbe's in its details. As Choi drew from Drabbe to supplement her own fieldwork, it is not always clear which elements of agreement are genuinely confirmational and which are mere repetitions. Final consonants are indicated in parentheses on Choi's attestations where the final is known (q.v. 2015: 31-33, see below for our reanalysis of Choi's proposed noun classes.) The comparisons which follow include only those terms which are found in both sources or have known outcomparisons to other Kolopom languages.

Bilabial nasal /m/ occurs initially and medially:

Drabbe Choi
m- m-
tie miˈa mia
some miˈta mitə
shoot ˈmindʏdʏra
one mɛːbʏ mɛβə
tongue mɛβʏ-dombo meβə-dəmbo
stone ˈmɛtʏ
rib(s) mʏrʏn-ʏmbo
flesh/meatmɛːratʏ mɛratə
breast ma
five mata mata
no/not mowo ~maˈwo mawo ~ mo
head moˈdo mɔdə
deep ˈmoa
vomit morʏ morə
eat/drink moˈra ~ -mʏramora
mother maˈka muka
breast mu mo “breast”
mu “milk”
birth mu mu
rain mi mɯi
husband mi -mɯi
-m- -m-
oblique -mɛ
louse nʏˈmɛ nəmə(n)
cheek tʲama-ˈtat
full komˈjɛti kəmia
cold rʏmada rəmo

Apical nasal /n/ occurs initially and medially:

Drabbe Choi
n- n-
1 pl. nom./poss.ni ni
1 pl. obj. ni-ɛː ni-ɛ
weep/cry niʏβʏrʏ ni-ni
tear(s) niːrʏ
dog niˈa nja(m) ~ ɲia
machete niˈarʏ
1 sg. obj. n-ɛː n-ɛ
bird nɛ ~ ne- nɛ(m)
sit nendʏn-dʏrɛnɛndəra
grandmother -neβa nɛwɛ
louse nʏˈmɛ nəmə(n)
spirit/ghost nʏmba(ː)
1 sg. nom./p. na na
rope naː na
name na®
sky/world (?) naˈndʲowja naɲɟəβia
know naːˈkɛ nakɛ
wallaby no no(n)
fly (v.) nomba nɔmba
star noːtʲʏ nɔcə
wind noˈambo nwambo
new/recent noˈana nwanana
-n- -n-
two ɛˈnaβa ɛnaβa
hold aˈmba-nʏna amba-nəna
daughter noˈa anwa
new/recent noˈana nwanana
bite ˈturaneβa turanɛwɛ
stand dinianʏ-na dinianə
live dʏnʏ-na dɛnɛ
axe kaːβuˈna
people ria-nˈtana ria-n̪d̪ana

Choi (2015: 19, 55) recognizes a phonemic velar nasal upon the basis of one word she gives as [ŋaβa] “in-law.” As this does not appear in Drabbe and has no currently known etymology, there is little we can say about this form besides noting that Kolopom had no phonemic /ŋ/, nor does one exist in neighboring Marianne Straia and Marind families, although there is an /ŋ/ in Yelmek which could conceivably be the source of a loan.

Bilabial voiceless stop /p/ is realized as unlenited [p] only root-initially:

DrabbeChoi
p-p-
three pɛndʏ pəndə
ground/belowpota- pɔtɛ-
~ pɔta-βota
house potʲʏ pocə
sago pu pu
long ˈpiβo pwiβɔ
~ pɯβɯ

Medial /p/ is lenited to voiced fricative [β] as it is in Kimaghama corresponding to Ndom /f/ just as does initial /p/:

Drabbe Choi
-p- [β b]-p- [β]
two ɛˈnaβa ɛnaβa
wing aˈβa aβwa(m)
see aˈβo aβo
hit/beat ˈowβura ɔβora
one mɛːbʏ mɛβə
tongue mɛβʏ-dombo meβə-dəmbo
long ˈpiβo pwiβɔ
~ pɯβɯ
sleepy -βaˈβo-
ground/below pɔta-βota
thumb/big toetiβɛ
hand taː-βat ta-βwatə
~ ta-βatə
pick up -ˈtraβʏ
belly ˈdi-βo di-βu
woman/wife duˈaβo d̪waβo
heel kaː-βʏ ka-βə
foot/sole ka-watʲʏ-pɛːtka-βatə
axe kaːβuˈna
bow kaˈraβo
hear roˈa-bar rwa-βarə
hair ru-tiβʏ ru-tiβə®

One apparent exception is also an exception in Kimaghama from the same rule, indicating a compound or a loan:

Drabbe Choi
-p- [p]
drytʏˈpanjɛn

Apical voiceless stop /t/ occurs initially and medially:

Drabbe Choi
t- t-
forehead ti ti
ashes/dust tin-aˈna ti(n)
thumb/big toetiβɛ
laugh tiˈti titi
foot tɛː
shin tɛrʏ
river
dry tʏˈpanjɛn
straight tʏmbʏra
six tʏrwa tərwa
speech tʏrwa- tʏrwa
~ trʏa-rʏa
tərwa-tərwa
hand taː-βat ta-βwatə
coconut shelltaːbo
finger taːn-ʏmbo tan-əmbo
arm ta-ka
swim ˈtatabʏ tatabi
many tarʏ tarə
hole to
tooth ˈtu-dʏmbo tu-dəmbo
elbow tudʏ tundə
bite ˈturaneβa turanɛwɛ
-t- -t-
stone ˈmɛtʏ
flesh/meat mɛːratʏ mɛratə
five mata mata
ground/below pota- pɔtɛ-
~ pɔta-βota
nose ˈbutʏ butə
laugh tiˈti titi
mouth ˈdota dotə
lip(s) tʲatʏ catə®
day rʏti rati
hair ru-tiβʏ ru-tiβə®

In one example, medial /t/ in a root meaning “sit down/lie down” is given as [d] when following /n/ in the preceding root:

Drabbe Choi
-t-
lie down pɔtɛ-təra
-t-/_n [d]-t-/_n [d]
sit nendʏn-dʏrɛ nɛndəra

Palatal voiceless stop /c/ occurs initially and medially:

Drabbe Choi
tʲ- c-
sweet tʲitʲʏ
knee tʲʏ cu
firewood tʲʏt-aˈna
thunder tʲʏdʏ
cheek tʲama-ˈtat
tail tʲambʏ cambə®
heart ˈtʲan-ʏmbʏcan-əmbo
lip(s) tʲatʏ catə®
cough/phlegm tʲaˈtʲa
break (stone)tʲaˈra
suck cu-mora
-tʲ- -c-
sneeze atʲi-ma
face ˈatʲʏ
house potʲʏ pocə
star noːtʲʏ nɔcə
sweet tʲitʲʏ
cough/phlegm tʲaˈtʲa
mountain ˈkarkatʲa karəkacə
sand rʏtʲi rəci
kunai grass ˈwatʲʏ

In addition palatal voiceless, plain voiced and prenasalized voiced stops, Drabbe (1949: 4) aasserts the existence of a fricative /s/' however only three examples, all medial, appear in his termlist and Choi's (2015) materials show Indonesian /s/ to be interpreted by Riandana spreakers as voiceless stop [tʲ c] (below.) Kolopom /*s/ is likewise regularly reflected as [tʲ c] in Riandana, suggesting [s] to be most likely an allophone of the palatal voiceless stop.

Velar voiceless stop /k/ occurs initially and medially:

Drabbe Choi
k- k-
arrow kiˈda
leaf kebe-dʏˈmbo kɛbə
heel kaː-βʏ ka-βə
foot/sole ka-watʲʏ-pɛːt ka-βatə
axe kaːβuˈna
toe kaː-dʏmb ka-dəmbo
snake ka ka
testicles ka ka
scabies kaˈmbi-kaˈmbi
call ˈkambo
sugarcane kaˈnta
banana kaːrʏ karə
urine kaː-ro-aˈna ka-rə
bow kaˈraβo
mountain ˈkarkatʲa karəkacə
full komˈjɛti kəmia
pig ku
penis ˈkodʏ kwəd̪ə®
skin/bark kwiˈka kwika
fruit/seed-kwa kwa
coconut kuˈa kwa
neck kwakʏ kwakə
root ˈkwaran-ʏˈmba kwɔrə
-k- -k-
child/son aˈka aka
mother maˈka muka
short mbʏka mbəka
know naːˈkɛ nakɛ
smoke didʏkwʏ didəkwə
die doˈakʏmbo dwakəmbo
bone ˈnduka n̪d̪uka®
father ntʲʏka ɲɟəka
skin/bark kwiˈka kwika
neck kwakʏ kwakə
intestinesroˈa-ka ~ roa-krwa-kə
think roˈa-ka-do rwa-kə-d̪u
ear ruˈa-kɛ rwa-kɛ
3 pl. nom.ˈjakwo jakwɔ
~ jakwə

Plain voiced stops and prenasalized voiced stops are dealt with together in the following section because the distinctions between them are marginal and of recent origin. The original rule looks to have been that prenasalized bilabial and apical /mb nd/ were realized as plain voiced [b d] in initial position, the situation for palatal and velar /ɲɟ ŋg/ being less clear. Such allophony is very common in New Guinean languages, sometimes leading to contrasts in compounds or where a destressed initial vowels have been lost. This is illustrated directly by Drabbe's [bˈjɛtʏ-mbˈjɛt] “pagaai (paddle),” whichi s plainly a reduplication, and by 3 sg poss. [ntʏ n̪d̪ə] where both internal and external comparision shows the former presence of an initial vowel [e ɛ]. Distributionally, prenasalized [mb nd] are far more common medially than are plain [b d] while initially the converse is true. Moreover, at least some examples of medial [b] are lenited reflexes of voiceless /p/ (above,) while several examples of medial [d] look to result from reduplication of initial [d] (below.)

Bilabial plain voiced stop /b/ …

Drabbe Choi
b- b-
fight/war baːdʏrʏ baɣədəra
nose ˈbutʏ butə
bamboo bɛː
-b- -b-
coconut shelltaːbo
swim ˈtatabʏ tatabi
leaf kebe-dʏˈmbokɛbə

Bilabial prenasalized voiced stop /mb/ …

Drabbe Choi
mb- mb-
short mbʏka mbəka
wash ˈmbari mbari
good mbuˈri mbori
-mb- -mb-
take aˈmba- amba/ɔmba
hold aˈmba-nʏna amba-nəna
quick amboj ambwɛ
spirit/ghostnʏmba(ː)
fly (v.) nomba nɔmba
wind noˈambo nwambo
straight tʏmbʏra
breathe dʏ-dʏmba -dəmbə
die doˈakʏmbo dwakəmbo
tail tʲambʏ cambə®
scabies kaˈmbi-kaˈmbi
call ˈkambo
nail wɛmbɛː-tʲʏ wɛmbɛ(n)
earth/groundrʏmbʏ -rəmbə
night/dark βe-rʏmbʏ wə-rəmbə

Choi asserts a phonemic distinction between apical voiced stops /d nd/ and dental voiced stops /d̪ n̪d̪/, but even the examples she chooses to illustrate this contrast (p. 19) are represented inconsistently in her data (pp. 19. 21 [d̪rə] “fire”, p. 57 term #190 [d̪rə-wa] “warm”, but p. 55, term #28 [drawarə] “burn”, term #62 [drə-nanakwɔ] “flames”, pp. 19, 22 [drə] “ship” but p. 44 [d̪rə] “ship”) as are a number of other words. As this distinction is unusual, is not recognized by Drabbe and would have no known origin, we tentatively conclude it to be non-phonemic. Choi adds that [d̪] has a “tense, near voiceless quality,” which suggests a connection to Drabbe's sporadic representation of /nd/ (?) as [nt]; however if so the correspondence is not consistent and in any event would apply only to /nd/ as Choi's [d] is never given as [t] in Drabbe.

Initial apical plain voiced stop /d/ is far more common than are initial /nd/ (below) or medial /d/ (below):

Drabbe Choi
d- [d]d- [d d̪]
sick/ill diβʏ
belly ˈdi-βo di-βu
stand dinianʏ-nadinianə
spine dintu dindu®
smoke didʏkwʏ didəkwə
fish diˈa dia(m)
live dʏnʏ-na dɛnɛ
fat/grease
breathe dʏ-dʏmba -dəmbə
game da
mouth ˈdota dotə®
bad ˈdodo d̪ɔd̪ɔ
die doˈakʏmbo dwakəmbo
smell/kissdu d̪u
fire drʏ d̪rə ~ drə
canoe drʏ drə ~ d̪rə

One example of variation between initials [d] and [nd] is explained by Choi (pp. 37-38) as resulting from the prefixation of a possessive element /n-/ which is not distinguished by person:

Drabbe Choi
d- [d] d- [d d̪]
womanduˈaβo d̪waβo
~ dwaβo
n-d- [nd]n-d- [nd]
wife n-doaːβo n-dwaβo

Medial /d/ is less common that medial /nd/ (below) and at least two examples (“smoke” and “bad”, see also “breathe” above) look to be reduplications, while the word for “garden” is found as /ma/ in Kimaghama and as /mar/ in Ndom, suggesting the Riandana root to be merely /ma/. “Penis” may also be a compound as Choi gives “vagina” as [kwəra(m)]:

Drabbe Choi
-d- [d]-d- [d d̪]
come eda ɛd̪a
shoot ˈmindʏdʏra
garden maˈda
head moˈdo mɔdə
fight/warbaːdʏrʏ baɣədəra
thunder tʲʏdʏ
smoke didʏkwʏ didəkwə
bad ˈdodo d̪ɔd̪ɔ
arrow kiˈda
penis ˈkodʏ kwəd̪ə®
think roˈaka-do rwakə-d̪u

Initial apical prenasalized voiced stop /nd/ is uncommon but appears to be irreducible. The third person singular possessive has lost an initial vowel, suggesting an origin for the contrast with /d/:

Drabbe Choi
nd- [nd nt]nd- [n̪d̪ nd]
make ndiˈwa n̪d̪iwa
3 sg. poss.ntʏ n̪d̪ə
bone ˈnduka n̪d̪uka®
tree/wood nduˈa n̪d̪wa ~ ndua

Medial /nd/ is more common that are initial /nd/ or medial /d/ (above):

Drabbe Choi
-nd- [nd nt]-nd- [nd n̪d̪]
3 sg. obl.enta ɛn̪d̪a
big aˈntɛ an̪d̪ɛ
shoot ˈmindʏdʏra
three pɛndʏ pəndə
sit nendʏn-dʏrɛ nɛndəra
spine dintu dindu®
sugarcane kaˈnta
four wɛːndʏ wɛndə
people ria-nˈtana ria-n̪d̪ana

One word is given with medial [d] in Drabbe but with medial [nd] in Choi. Comparison to Drabbe's (!949: 15) Kimaghama and Geurtjens' (1933: 400-401) Kaladdarsch and Teri Kalwasch forms suggests it to be a compound with the root /tu(n)/:

Drabbe Choi
-d- [d]-nd- [nd]
elbowtudʏ tundə

Palatal plain voiced stop [ɟ] is atttested in only three words, Drabbe's [dʲadʲʏ-kwakʏ] “larynx (ademsappel)” and [dʲaː] “boeren (belch)”, though Drabbe does not include it in his phonemic inventory, and Choi's (p. 19) [ɟa] “to farm” and [ɟaɟə kwakə] “adam's apple”. Choi's gloss “to farm” is probably a mistranslation of Drabbe's “boeren” which also means “farmer (pl.)”, suggesting that her versions are not the result of fieldwork but repetitions drawn from Drabbe's termlist. The word for “larynx” is prefixed to “neck” and is probably a reduplication of “belch”. As these words have no known etymologies we can say little about the origin of [ɟ]:

Drabbe Choi
dʲ- ɟ-
belch dʲaː ɟa
larynxdʲadʲʏ-kwakʏɟaɟə kwakə
-dʲ- -ɟ-
larynxdʲadʲʏ-kwakʏɟaɟə kwakə

Initial palatal prenasalized voiced stop /ɲɟ/ is found only in second person plural pronouns, where historically it reflects a palatalized version of velar /ŋg/ (below.) It seems possible that this should really be analyzed as a cluster /ŋgj/ as such clusters /Cj/ are known to exist in initial position (below):

Drabbe Choi
ndʲ-ɲɟ-
2 pl. obj.ntʲ-ɛː ɲɟ-ɛ
2 pl. nom.ntʲʏ ɲɟə

Medial palatal prenasalized voiced stop /ɲɟ/ is uncommon. Unlike initial /ɲɟ/, outcomarisons of “lightning”, which is also found in the Marianne Strait languages (where palatalization of apical /nd/ is automatic when folowed by /i/,) provide no indication of an origin as velar /ŋg/. However, it likewise seems possible that these should really be analyzed as clusters /ndj/ as this sound is far less common than voiceless /c/ (above) and besides “lightning” is not established medially in proto-Kolopom:

Drabbe Choi
-ndʲ- -ɲɟ-
sky/world (?)naˈndʲowjanaɲɟəβia
lightning ˈwandʲʏ

Velar plain voiced stop [g] is atttested in only three words, Drabbe's [aˈga] “zoete bataat (sweet potato)”, though Drabbe does not include it in his phonemic inventory, and Choi's [gamɔ] “(pig) tusk” and [pagə] “torso”. As neither have known etymologies, it's not possible to draw any firm conclusions besides noting that both Drabbe and Choi assert that a phoneme /g/ exists. It is interesting to note though that Indonesian /guru/ “teacher” was borrowed as [ŋgurə] (2015: 43, 53.)

Velar prenasalized voiced stop /ŋg/ has been found only initially in second person singular pronouns. As nominative and oblique (but not possessive) forms are found with preceding initial vowels in closely-related Kimaghama but not in Ndom or Moraori, it's difficult to say whether they were originally root-initial:

Drabbe Choi
ŋg-ŋg-
2 sg. nom. ŋgʏ ŋgə
2 sg. poss.ŋga ŋga

Choi's velar voiced fricative [ɣ] is given in six words where it occurs only medially. One of these has a counterart in Drabbe where the sound is not recognized at all:

Drabbe Choi
-ø--ɣ-
fight/warbaːdʏrʏbaɣədəra

It seems possible though somewhat counterintuitive that this was originally the medial reflex of /ŋg/ which is otherwise not attested, and would not be inconsistent with Drabbe's [aˈga] “zoete bataat (sweet ptoato)” as noted above though would seem to contradict Choi's (p. 19) assertion of a contrast between [paɣə] “crocodile cave” and [pagə] “torso”. Alternatively, they are loans from neighboring Ndom in which [ɣ] is the regular reflex of both [*k *ŋg] (there is a /*ɣ / in both Bulaka River and in Marind but it has become /ŋ/ in Yelmek and /h/ both in Maklew and in the western dialects of Marind ).

Bilabial non-stop /w/ occurs initially and medially, sometimes being interpreted as voiced fricative [β] in Drabbe's vocabulary:

Drabbe Choi
w- [w β] w-
egg ˈwin-ʏmb-ana wi(n-əmbo)
dream
nail wɛmbɛː-tʲʏ wɛmbɛ(n)
four wɛːndʏ wɛndə
thigh βeˈrimo wɛri
night/dark βerʏmbʏ wərəmbə
kunai grassˈwatʲʏ
lightning ˈwandʲʏ
sleep (n.) ˈβia- βia- ~ βiɛ-
-w- [w β]-w-
cassowary aˈwi awi
upper arm aˈwa awa®
no/not mowo ~ maˈwo mawo ~ mo
grandmother-neβa nɛwɛ
bite ˈturaneβa turanɛwɛ
sick/ill diβʏ
cook/burn draˈwaro drawarə
make ndiˈwa n̪d̪iwa

Apical non-stop /r/ occurs initially and medially:

Drabbe Choi
r- r-
bathe riˈra rira
person ria ria
water rʏ ~ ˈru-ana
ro-aˈna
cold rʏmada rəmo
earth/ground rʏmbʏ -rəmbə
sand rʏtʲi rəci
day rʏti rati
black ˈru-datari ru
leech ru ru(n)
hair ru-tiβʏ ru-tiβə®
body hair ˈrur-ana run-əmbo
hear roˈa-bar rwa-βarə
ear ruˈa-kɛ rwa-kɛ
intestines roˈa-ka ~ roa-k rwa-kə
think roˈa-ka-do rwa-kə-d̪u
-r- -r-
hit/beat ˈowβura ɔβora
shoot ˈmindʏdʏra
rib(s) mʏrʏn-ʏmbo
flesh/meat mɛːratʏ mɛratə
vomit morʏ morə
eat/drink moˈra ~ -mʏra mora
fight/war baːdʏrʏ baɣədəra
wash ˈmbari mbari
good mbuˈri mbori
machete niˈarʏ
tear(s) niːrʏ
shin tɛrʏ
straight tʏmbʏra
lie down -təra
sit nendʏn-dʏrɛ nɛndəra
six tʏrwa tərwa
speech tʏrwa- tʏrwa
~ trʏa-rʏa
tərwa-tərwa
many tarʏ tarə
bite ˈturaneβa turanɛwɛ
break (stone)tʲaˈra
cook/burn draˈwaro drawarə
banana kaːrʏ karə
bow kaˈraβo
mountain ˈkarkatʲa karəkacə
root ˈkwaran-ʏˈmba kwɔrə
bathe riˈra rira

Palatal non-stop [j] has been clearly found only initially:

Drabbe Choi
j- j-
give ĩ ji
blood jɛr-aˈnajə®
saliva/spittlejʏr-aˈnajɛ-tə
3 sg. nom.
3 pl. nom. ˈjakwo jakwɔ
~ jakwə
white (?) jwɛ

Consonant clusters consist of a consonant followed by one of three non-stops /w r j/ and occur initially and medially.

In the examples which follow, Drabbe's phonetic vowels [o u] followed by another vowel are in free variation, as may be seen in “woman/wife” and the root for “thought/feelings” /rwa/ which is the base of “hear”, “ear”, “intestines” (glossed also as “like” in Choi 2015: 43, cf. also Drabbe's 1949: 16 [rʏ moˈra roak] “dorstig (thirsty)”, i.e. “water eat desire”) and “think”, and correspond to Choi's [w]. We follow Choi in interpreting this sound as medial bilabial non-stop /w/ rather than as a vowel sequence /uV oV/. In addition to the fact that clusters involving other non-stops as second members /*Cr *Cj/ exist (below,) one point in favor of an interpretation as a cluster is that Drabbe hears the second vowel as stressed:

Drabbe Choi
Cw-
[Cu Co Cw]
Cw- [Cw]
wind noˈambo nwambo
new/recentnoˈana nwanana
die doˈakʏmbo dwakəmbo
woman/wifeduˈaβo
~ doaːβo
d̪waβo
skin/bark kwiˈka kwika
fruit/seed-kwa kwa
coconut kuˈa kwa
neck kwakʏ kwakə
root ˈkwaran-ʏˈmba kwɔrə
hear roˈa-bar rwa-βarə
ear ruˈa-kɛ rwa-kɛ
intestinesroˈa-ka ~ roa-k rwa-kə
think roˈa-ka-do rwa-kə-d̪u
white (?) jwɛ

Clusters with bilabial initials /mw pw/ have been identified before high front vowel /i/, where the existence of /w/ is not recognized by Drabbe:

Drabbe Choi
mi-mwi- [mɯ]
rain mi mɯi
husbandmi -mɯi
pi-pwi- [pwi pɯ]
long ˈpiβo pwiβɔ
~ pɯβɯ

… /pw/ …:

Drabbe Choi
pw- [po]
mousepoˈar

Outcomparison to both Kimaghama and Ndom of one example in which both Drabbe and Choi give initial /mb/ shows this to represent cluster /bw/:

Drabbe Choi
bw [b]bw [b]
bamboobɛː

Medial clusters with bilabials /pw mbw/ have also been found:

Drabbe Choi
-pw- [β b w]-pw- [βw β]
wing aˈβa aβwa(m)
dig -oba oβwa
hand taː-βat ta-βwatə
~ ta-βatə
foot/soleka-watʲʏ-pɛːt ka-βatə
-mbo- -mbw-
quick amboj ambwɛ

Drabbe (1949: 3-4) posits phonemes /tr dr/, Initial /tr/ is found in at least one root (p. 220 [amba- ˈtraβʏ] “gaan halen (fetch)”, [awarʏ- ˈtraβʏ] “meenemen (pick up)”, cf. [amba-nʏna] “vastpakken (grab)”, [awarʏ-treda] “meebringen (bring)”) in which it corresponds to Kimaghama /tr/:

Drabbe Choi
tr-
pick up-ˈtraβʏ

Another example of initial [tr] is given elsewhere as [tʏr]:

Drabbe Choi
tʏr- ~ trʏ- tər-
speechtʏrwa- tʏrwa
~ trʏa-rʏa
tərwa-tərwa

Somewhat better established is initial /dr/, which we follow Choi in analyzing as a cluster:

Drabbe Choi
dr-dr- [dr d̪r]
fire drʏ d̪rə ~ drə
canoedrʏ drə ~ d̪rə

There is no direct evidence for a medial /j/ as there is for medial /w/, which besides occuring between two non-high vowels is often heard as occluded in Drabbe (above.) The following examples are interpreted as /j/ as the second member of a cluster analogous to those involving /w r/ (above) or as sequential vowels. One point in favor of an interpretation as a cluster is that Drabbe hears the second vowel as stressed:

Drabbe Choi
Cj- [Ci]Cj [Ci Cj]-
tie miˈa mia
dog niˈa nja(m) ~ ɲia
machete niˈarʏ
fish diˈa dia(m)
sleep (n.) ˈβia- βia- ~ βiɛ-
person ria ria
sky/world (?)naˈndʲowja naɲɟəβia
stand dinianʏ-na dinianə
full komˈjɛti kəmia

Generally, Kolopom final consonants are dropped in Riandana as they are in Kimaghama, but finals /m n r/ and possibly /t/ are still present in the underlying forms of the roots and can surface when the root is followed by one of several suffixes. Choi (2015: 31-33) analyzes these finals as prefixes to the following suffix, drawing an analogy to prefixed verbal objects (p. 39,) and states that they are governed by the class of the preciding nominal, even impuitng a semantic significance to these “noun classes.” Unlike object prefixed verbs, which can appear in a number of syntantic positions, these allegedly agreement-inflected forms invariably follow the nominals they modify; i.e. they are suffixes, not independent words as Choi presents them. Outcomparison to Ndom, which does not drop final consonants in free forms, show them to be phonological in origin:

RiandanaNdom
Choi Drabbe
(m) m
wing aβwa(m) xaˈfam
(n) n
louse nəmə(n) nɛːmʏn
dust/ashesti(n) -ˈtin
® θ
blood jə® ɛθ
® r
name na® nar

The following examples illustrate contrasts between roots with final consonants and those followed by the suffixes [-ana] collective, for which Choi (2015: 32) gives the equivalent [-andwa], and [-(d)ʏmbo -(d)əmbo] of uncertain meaning:

Drabbe Choi
ø-ana ø-andwa
egg ˈwin-ʏmb-ana
water ˈru-ana
ro-aˈna
rəw-andwa
-n-ana -n-andwa
ashes/dust tin-aˈna tin-andwa
-t-ana
firewood tʲʏt-aˈna
-r-ana -r-andwa
body hair ˈrur-ana
blood jɛr-aˈna jər-andwa
saliva/spittlejʏr-aˈna
-ø-dʏmbo -ø-dəmbo
tongue mɛβʏ-dombo meβə-dəmbo
tooth ˈtu-dʏmbo tu-dəmbo
anus ˈdi-dʏmbʏ
leaf kebe-dʏˈmbo kɛbə
toe kaː-dʏmb ka-dəmbo
-n-ʏmbo -n-əmbo
eye ˈan-ʏmbʏ
~ an-ʏmba
an-əmbo
rib(s) mʏrʏn-ʏmbo
finger taːn-ʏmbo tan-əmbo
pinkie taːn-aˈnan-
-ʏmbo
heart ˈtʲan-ʏmbʏ can-əmbo
banana karən-əmbo
root ˈkwaran-ʏˈmba
egg ˈwin-ʏmb-ana win-əmbo
-n-əmbo
body hair run-əmbo

Drabbe and Choi agree that Riandana has seven vowels, but disgree about precisely what these are, with Drabbe asserting a contrast between high and low mid front vowels [e ɛ] and Choi between high and low mid back vowels [o ɔ].

High front vowel /i/ …

DrabbeChoi

High back rounded vowel …

DrabbeChoi

Central vowel … <ö> … The same grapheme in Kimaghama is said to signify /œ/ … Drabbe's choice of a rounded symbol rather than e.g. <ĕ> to be well-considered …. Choi says mothing about the character of this sound besides that it is a mid central vowel, with the choice of <ə> suggesting an unrounded sound …

DrabbeChoi

Occasionally it is given as <o>, possibly due to misprints, but the example of “water” when followed by the collective suffix /-ana/ …

DrabbeChoi

Mid front vowel …

DrabbeChoi

Mid back rounded vowel … /o ɔ/ … Choi's (2015: 21) [pɔta-βota] “lie down” is particuarly relavant as it is clearly a reduplication of the root meaning “ground/below”, yet the vowel is given differently in each iteration …

Drabbe Choi
o o
house potʲʏ pocə
eat/drink moˈra ~ -mʏramora
mouth ˈdota dotə®
see aˈβo aβo
woman/wife duˈaβo d̪waβo
o oː ɔ
bad ˈdodo d̪ɔd̪ɔ
head moˈdo mɔdə
hit/beat ˈowβura ɔβora
star noːtʲʏ nɔcə
fly (v.) nomba nɔmba
ground/belowpota- pɔtɛ-
~ pɔta-βota

Low cental vowel /a/ …

DrabbeChoi

Pronouns

[under construction]

Drabbe (1949: 5-6) gives Riantana pronouns in three case forms. Choi (2015: 37-38) reanalyzes some of these into four cases as follows:

Drabbe Drabbe Drabbe Choi Choi Choi Choi
nominativeoblique possessivesubject/agent object/patientpossessiveoblique
1 sg.na nɛː na na na ?
2 sg.ŋgʏ ŋgɛː ŋga ŋgə ŋga ŋga ?
3 sg.enta ntʏ ŋgɛ n̪d̪ə ɛn̪d̪a
1 pl.ni niɛː ni ni niɛ ni nia
2 pl.ntʲʏ ntʲɛː ntʲʏ ɲɟə ɲɟɛ ? ɲɟa
3 pl.ˈjakwo ˈjakwotaˈjakwʏtʏ jakwɔ
~ jakwə
? ? jakwɔta


Verbal morphology

[under construction]

Like Kimaghama and Ndom, Riandana stands out within Trans New Guinea in being an isolating language with almost no affixing verbal morphology, a type which Drabbe (1950: 548) labeled “Koppelwoord talen” because categories are distinguished by adposition. There is a purposive suffix /-tə/ but it is clearly the same as the allative suffix on nominals and is probably a clitic as it is sometimes given as an independent word (q.v. Choi 2015: 29-30. 48.)

Choi (2015: 47-49, 59-50) gives tense and mood auxilliaries for Riantana as follows:

past wata
near past arəta
present ø/ndi
near futureɛn̪d̪ɛ ~ ɛn̪d̪-ə
future aka
subjunctiveka/kati
adhortativeama
irrealis βwa
prohibitivembi

Most of these auxilliaries are preposed not only to verbs but to predicates which typically consist of an absolutive nominal followed by a verb. …

Choi (2015: 29-30) gives examples of verbs the stems of which change vowels to indicate realis or irrealis mood; here it seems likely that realis forms were once prefixed with a vowel [o ɔ] which has metathsized into the stem, with [ɛ] in “sleep” induced by adjacent /j/:

irrealisrealis
take amba ɔmba
wake namba nɔmba
hit aβora oβora
sleepβjatəra βjɛtəra
~ βiɛtəra

As with many other Trans New Guinea languages, there are prefixes which indicate the object of the verb; however the Riandana prefixes bears no resmblance to those of any other known system. These prefixes surface only before vowel-initial auxilliaries Choi 2015: 39):

1/2 r-
3 sg.n-
3 pl.ø-

Loans from neighboring languages

[under construction]



At least two roots look to have been borrowed from Marind to the east, though as these words have no known etymologies in Marind-Yakhai or in Fly River the direction of borrowing might be disputed:

Marind Drabbe Choi
moon *mandeumaˈnta
body hair*ɾuɾ ˈrur-anarun-əmbo

Another word may be cognate but its phonology suggests the Riandana word to be original, as [β] is the medial allophone of /p/ (above) while Marind /*h/ descends from Fly River /*ɸ/ and this Marind word, which is found also in Moraori as [jɛna-du] (Drabbe 1954: 68) where the loss of Kolopom medial /*p/ woudl be regular, otherwise has no known etymology:

MarindDrabbeChoi
two*inah ɛˈnaβaɛnaβa

A number of loans from Indonesian are found especially in Choi's texts. The retention of [h] in “year” suggests that this may not even be a proper loan but multilingualism on the part of Choi's narrator. Otherwise Indonesian sounds are assimilated to the native phonology, as is especailly conspicuous in the interpretation of [s] as [tʲ c]:

IndonesianDrabbe Choi
knife pisaᵘ petʲʏˈɛ
year tahun tahun
school səkolah cakwara
work kərdʒa kərɛɟa
teacherguru ŋgurə®

Another loan is presumably from Indonesian via an unknown local source and is found in other Kolopom languages, Bulaka River, Marind and others in more or less identical form (q.v. Drabbe 1949: 18, 1950: 570, 1954: 134, etc.) Choi's medial [β] is unexpected and possibly in error:

IndonesianDrabbe Choi
tobaccotəmbakaᵘ ˈtamukuʈamuβə